5 quick PR tricks to cover up a lack of real health care news

By | January 26, 2019

As all researchers know, science is a grinding parade of failure and dead ends. But as we’ve often written, news release writers sometimes seem hell-bent on making the public believe otherwise.

Like expert makeup artists, they can add sparkle to lackluster findings, mask blemishes in study designs, and smooth over unimpressive data.

One thing I won’t miss much about my job at HealthNewsReview.org as it ends this month is reading the daily churn of PR releases.

Every weekday morning our associate editor, Kat Stone, has scanned the internet for news releases about research on medical interventions.

What’s striking is how rarely these announcements reveal something of real consequence for consumers. On many days in many ways, they tout findings that are inconclusive.

On some days our staff selected a news release to review if the topic was likely to spark public interest. Most of the time, though, we’ve just groaned and moved on.

As a final exercise, I’ll treat you (as briefly as possible) to Thursday’s entire batch of five news releases, which Stone called a “typical array of poor-to-average releases.”

Each offers a lesson on how mundane findings can be prettied up with a stroke of PR magic.

1) Tout “potential.”

Nebraska virologists discover safer potential Zika vaccine (University of Nebraska-Lincoln)

We see this word a lot: “potential.” In medical science, it’s pretty meaningless. I can safely say that my beloved mutt has potential to star in a movie some day. (After all, she looks exactly like that poopy dog in “Roma.”) Unfortunately, it’s unlikely that she will.

Claims are especially tenuous when they come from animal studies. A subhead in this release says this “new mechanism could be a ‘huge leap’ for immunology,” with no mention until the 13th paragraph that this was only tested in mice.

See also  EHR vendors say ONC proposed rules are too broad, would stifle innovation

2) Skip the data.

Performance enhancer: Sports compression stockings a winning advantage (James Cook University)

This release doesn’t give any data to support the idea that compression socks will help you (or your kid) perform better on the soccer field. In fact, the study involved testing just 20 players during a single soccer match and measuring whether the 10 who wore compression socks performed better on agility, heel-rise, and endurance tests after the game.

A researcher is quoted saying the study “confirmed that there is a protective effect with compression stockings that may be crucial for performance in soccer matches.” But where are the numbers to show the scope of this purported benefit?

3) Pose a question.

Can Facebook advertising prevent cancer?  (University of Colorado Anschutz Medical Campus)

Spoiler alert: We don’t know.

As we’ve written, question-mark headlines can be used to skirt accountability for reporting an unproven claim.

This study showed that Facebook was a “good mechanism” to convey messages about cancer screening. A researcher states: “Engagement was high with Facebook ads, and those who viewed ads clicked through to the sign-up page, an indication of intent to enroll.”

In the last paragraph we learn: “Technically, the answer to whether Facebook advertising can prevent cancer remains unanswered – it’s impossible to tell how many patients who otherwise would have developed cancer were caught early due to the group’s text-based information program.”

4) “Suggest” something.

Diabetes drug could be used to treat common heart failure syndrome, study suggests  (Rockefeller University Press)

The word “suggest” is a handy way to obscure that a claim isn’t proven. As we learn in the second-to-last paragraph, the drug — metformin — appeared to have an effect on mice with heart failure symptoms, not humans. 

See also  Phillips' Colon Health Daily Probiotic Supplement, 60 Count

Unmentioned is that previous research has similarly indicated metformin may help people with heart failure, but it’s an idea that has yet to be shown conclusively in a randomized clinical trial.

5) Use “may.”

In Just Six Months, Exercise May Help Those with Thinking Problems (American Academy of Neurology)

Similarly, “may” is a word that masks uncertainty.

We’ve often advised readers that whenever they see “may” in a claim about a study result, feel free to substitute “may not.” Chances are good that you’d end up being right.

I don’t want to be too hard on this news release, which can be credited with explaining how this randomized trial at Duke University was conducted and emphasizing that results need to be interpreted cautiously due to the small number of participants.

But in using the word “may” no fewer than five times, it obscures the point — made near the end of the release — that bigger studies involving multiple trial sites are needed to prove that exercise sustainably improves executive function in adults at risk for cognitive decline.

Exaggeration is common

You might be tempted to think that yesterday’s batch of incautious releases — four out of five of which were issued by a university — was an anomaly, and that academic institutions usually take utmost care in communicating medical research to the public.

But check out a 2009 paper by Drs. Steve Woloshin and Lisa Schwartz of the The Dartmouth Institute, which scrutinized 200 news releases issued by academic medical centers.

They found that news releases from academic institutions often exaggerated findings:

  • Forty-four percent promoted animal or laboratory research, of which 74% explicitly claimed relevance to human health.
  • Of releases about human research, 23% omitted study size and 34% didn’t quantify results.
  • Only 17% of releases on human research promoted studies with the strongest designs (randomized trials or meta-analyses).
  • Forty percent reported on the most limited human studies—those with uncontrolled interventions, small samples of less than 30 participants, surrogate outcomes, or unpublished data — with 58% lacking relevant cautions.
See also  True Health Dr. Cutler's Ultimate Colon Care Formula, with Chinese Rhubarb, Cape Aloe, Digestive Enzyme Blend

Our catalogue of more than 600 news release reviews shows that universities on the whole — and other organizations that issue news releases — haven’t mended their ways.

For a closer look on what consumers and journalists should watch out for when they read news releases, go to our recent series on problematic PR releases.

“The quickest strategy for improvement,” Woloshin and Schwartz wrote, “would be for centers to issue fewer releases about preliminary research, especially unpublished scientific meeting presentations, because findings often change substantially—or fail to hold up—as studies mature.”

We agree, the spigot could use tightening.

You might also like

HealthNewsReview.org